Can beauty concerns promote positive body image?
In light of research (e.g. Williams et al.,
2013) indicating that beauty concerns can be utilised to reduce unhealthy
behaviours such as tanning, smoking and more recently, alcohol consumption, a
central question remains about the ethics of doing so. In other words, in a
society where appearance increasingly defines who you are, and unrealistic
beauty standards are a distinct marker of social class (Grogan, 2016), is it
responsible of researchers to further emphasize this?
Beauty, and particularly youthfulness, has long
been the main standard on which women (and increasingly men, albeit not to the
same extent) are judged (Wolf, 1991), but perhaps never as intensively as in a
time where media images are omnipresent. Walter (2010) goes as far as suggesting
the 21st century to be the era of “new sexism” - related, but not
identical to, Glick & Fiske’s (1996) concept of Benevolent Sexism - where
traditional aspects of patriarchy such as objectification and sexualisation of
women are re-branded as empowering personal choices, perpetuated by women
themselves. Women are strongly discouraged from any form of natural ageing,
with plastic surgery offering a very real option to do so (Grogan, 2016). Given
what Self-Objectification Theory (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997) suggests
about the detrimental effects highlighting women’s appearance can have on
everything from their mental health to math’s performance, it is crucial that
researchers who utilise concerns about beauty and aging in research, strive to
do so in an ethical manner.
Within body image research, the focus is
increasingly shifting away from a traditional perspective of negative body
image to the promotion of a positive body image, among young people as well as
adults (Wood-Barcalow et al. 2010). Research on whole-body scanning has
indicated that technical solutions can be used to increase body satisfaction,
particularly among women, although long-term results are inconclusive (Grogan
et al., 2013). Looking at appearance-focused behaviour change techniques such
as facial morphing, is it possible that this could be utilised in a way to
promote positive, rather than negative perceptions of ageing? A crucial aspect
of the facial-morphing technique is that participants receive two aged photos:
one which is based on the health-damaging behaviour in question (e.g. smoking
or UV exposure) and a comparison photo based on natural ageing (without UV
smoking or UV exposure). Preliminary results of research on attitudes to UV
exposure and facial morphing among older women that is currently being
conducted at Manchester Metropolitan University indicates that the comparison
between the two photos can result in a positive evaluation of the natural aging
process. As well as expressing negative evaluations of the UV-exposed photos,
it appears that this participant group was pleasantly surprised by the
naturally aged photo, indicating that they felt positively about the way they
are likely to look when they're older. In addition to the comparison aspect, it
is also possible that facial-morphing instils people with a sense of
self-agency and self-efficacy, i.e. that their future appearance can be
directly influenced by themselves (i.e. using sun protection) rather than
aspiring to a beauty ideal presented in mainstream media that is largely
unattainable (Grogan, 2016). This is incredibly encouraging, as it suggests
that appearance-focused interventions such as facial morphing can be a positive
influence, not only in prompting behaviour change and thus increasing chances
of being physically healthy when older, but also in promoting a positive outlook
on the aging process itself.
By Sofia Persson
Sofia Persson is a PhD researcher and associate lecturer at Manchester Metropolitan University. Her primary research interests include behaviour change techniques and body image among men and women.
Fredrickson,
B. & Roberts, T. (1998). ‘The swimsuit becomes you: sex differences in
self-objectification, restrained eating, and math performance’. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
75(1) 269-284.
Glick,
P. & Fiske, S. (1996). ‘The ambivalent sexism inventory: differentiating
hostile and benevolent sexism’. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(3), 491-512.
Grogan, S.
(2016). Body image. London: Routledge
Grogan, S., Gill,
S., Brownbridge, K., Kilgariff, S. & Whalley, A. (2013) ‘Dress fit and body
image: A thematic analysis of women’s accounts during and after trying on
dresses’. Body Image. 10(3), 380-388.
Walter, N.
(2010). ‘Living dolls – the return of sexism’. London: Virago Press.
Williams, A., Grogan, S., Clark-Carter, D., Buckley, E. (2013). ‘Appearance-based interventions to reduce ultraviolet exposure and/or increase sun protection intentions and behaviours: A systematic review and meta-analysis’. British Journal of Health Psychology, 18(1), 182-217
Wolf, N. (1990),
The beauty myth. London: Vintage
Wood-Barclow,
N., Tylka, T. & Augustus-Horvavth, C. (2010) ‘But I like my Body’ Body Image, 7(3), 106-118
Wonderful blog! Everyone is beautiful in their own way. Visit for better treatment for skin and hair, click Best Dermatologist in East Delhi
ReplyDelete