How breast size dissatisfaction affects breast self-examination
Breast
cancer is the most common cancer diagnosed in the United Kingdom. In 2015, 45,764
cases of breast cancer in women were registered in England, accounting for
nearly 1 in 3 new female
cancer cases. Women with breast cancer in the United Kingdom also have poorer survival
rates than women in other
parts of Europe. One reason for this is that British women tend
to be diagnosed at more advanced
stages of disease, which reduces survival rates.
Regular
attendance at mammography
screenings is an effective way of detecting breast cancer early. In
England, women between the ages of 50 and 70 are invited for breast screening
every three years as part of the NHS
Breast Screening Programme. Screening is intended to detect breast
cancer at an early stage, when there is a better chance of successful
treatment. However, most breast cancers are detected
by women through breast self-examination, which involves regular
palpation of the breasts.
However,
done on its own and without proper training, breast self-examination may not
provide any
clear benefit for early breast cancer detection. In the UK,
healthcare policy no longer advocates regular breast self-examination and
instead promotes “breast
awareness”, which involves familiarity with one’s breasts and the
way the breasts change throughout one’s life. It involves women gaining an awareness of
how their breasts look and feel normally, as well as developing the confidence
to notice any change (e.g., changes in size, swelling, pain, lumps or
thickening) that might help
detect breast cancer early.
A
woman’s body image – or their subjective perceptions of their bodies – may be
an important
factor that affects breast awareness. This is because breast
self-examination requires physical examination of, and attentiveness to, one’s
own body. But the few studies that have examined this possibility have been inconclusive,
with some studies finding that negative body image is associated with lower
frequency of breast self-examination and others finding no
association. These mixed findings may be due to the fact
that studies have focused on women’s dissatisfaction with their overall bodies,
rather than breast dissatisfaction specifically.
A
colleague and I wanted to know whether breast size dissatisfaction affects
breast self-examination. To do this, we asked an online sample of 384 British
women to complete measures of breast size dissatisfaction and more general body
dissatisfaction. We also asked how frequently they engaged in breast
self-examination, how confident they felt about detecting a change in their
breasts, and how quickly they would see their doctor if they did detect a
change.
In our
study,
published in the journal Body Image, we
found that a majority of our participants were dissatisfied with their breast
size, with 31% wanting smaller breasts than they currently had and 44% wanting
larger breasts than they currently had. In terms of breast self-examination, a
third never examined their breasts, about a fifth self-examined once every six
months, about a quarter once a month, and about a fifth at least once a week.
More than a half of participants were not at all or only slightly confident of
detecting a breast change, though just over a half said they would visit their
doctor as soon as possible if they did detect a change.
Importantly,
our findings showed that women who experienced greater breast size
dissatisfaction, but not body dissatisfaction more generally, were less likely
to engage in frequent breast self-examination. Our results also showed that
both women who desired larger and smaller breasts than they currently had were
less likely to engage in regular breast self-examination. Women with greater
breast size dissatisfaction were also less confident about detecting a change
in their breasts and were more likely to delay seeing their doctor if they did
detect a change. In short, it seems to be dissatisfaction with one’s breasts
specifically, rather than one’s body as a whole, that has an influence on
breast self-examination frequency and related behaviours.
The
most likely explanation for our findings is that, for women who experience
breast size dissatisfaction, breast self-examination acts as a threat
to body image. Breast self-examination involves visually
inspecting each breast and feeling each breast to detect
changes. For women who are dissatisfied with their breasts, having to visually
and manually inspect their breasts may trigger negative body-related thoughts.
To avoid such negative thoughts, women with breast size dissatisfaction may
avoid performing self-examination. In other words, women who experience breast
size dissatisfaction may avoid performing breast self-examination because it
draws attention to the site
of their dissatisfaction.
Breast
size dissatisfaction may also trigger negative self-conscious emotions, such as
shame and embarrassment, that result in lower breast awareness. As Cornelia
Baines once put it,
breast self-examination “neither makes one feel better for having done it nor
makes one more attractive”. Instead, breast self-examination – which asks women
to ritually investigate whether they have been “betrayed” by
their breasts – may trigger negative feelings about the body because it forces
women to treat their bodies as objects.
Our
data were cross-sectional, so causal inferences should be drawn with caution,
and we only focused on a single aspect of breast dissatisfaction. Even so, our
findings suggest that promoting breast size satisfaction may be one way of
increasing breast self-examination frequency and more positive behaviours in
response to detecting breast change. For example, there is some evidence that
promoting breast
awareness helps women to view their breasts in more functional,
rather than purely aesthetic terms, which in turn may empower them to take a
more active role in breast self-examination. Until such interventions become
available, healthcare professionals should be mindful of the impact that
dissatisfaction with one’s breasts may have on self-examination behaviours.
Viren Swami is Professor of Social Psychology at Anglia Ruskin University. His research on human appearance and body image is focused on the influence of culture on beauty ideals an practices, as well as the socio-political underpinnings of corporeal experiences. He is the author of The Missing Arms of Venus de Milo, The Psychology of Physical Attraction, and Attraction Explained: The Science of How We Form Relationships.
Comments
Post a Comment